Absolute threshold
Absolute threshold is a concept in neuroscience and psychology that defines the minimum level of sensory input that can be detected by an organism. Specifically, it refers to the least amount of energy from a stimulus—such as light, sound, temperature, taste, or touch—that can be perceived at least 50% of the time. This threshold varies across different sensory modalities and can be influenced by factors like prior exposure to stimuli and the individual's expectations. The term was originally introduced in the 1890s by psychologists Ernst Heinrich Weber and Gustav Fechner, who focused on measuring sensory perception and the relationships between different levels of stimuli.
Researchers use various methods to determine absolute thresholds, including the adjustment method, limits method, constant stimuli method, and the staircase method, each designed to accommodate different experimental conditions. For instance, the adjustment method may require participants to adjust a stimulus until it is barely detectable, while the staircase method adjusts stimulus levels based on participant feedback. Additionally, absolute thresholds can fluctuate due to daily variations, health status, or conscious attention to specific stimuli, exemplified by phenomena like "nose blindness." Understanding absolute thresholds is essential for applications in fields like design and safety, where visibility and detectability are critical.
On this Page
Absolute threshold
Absolute threshold is a term in neuroscience and psychological research that refers to the least amount of sensory input that can be detected. Researchers usually further refine the absolute threshold as the least amount of energy output by a stimulus such as light or temperature that can be detected at least 50 percent of the time. This allows for the variety of factors that can affect the perception of a stimulus. The threshold can apply to sound, sight, taste, or touch and can be determined for any living organism. The threshold can also be changed by exposure to certain stimuli, expectation of perceiving it, and other factors.
![Absolute thresholds of hearing for male (M) and female (W) subjects between the ages of 20 and 60. The original uploader was Tlotoxl at English Wikipedia. [GFDL (http://www.gnu.org/copyleft/fdl.html) or CC-BY-SA-3.0 (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-sa/3.0/)], via Wikimedia Commons rssalemscience-20180712-2-171808.jpg](https://imageserver.ebscohost.com/img/embimages/ers/sp/embedded/rssalemscience-20180712-2-171808.jpg?ephost1=dGJyMNHX8kSepq84xNvgOLCmsE2epq5Srqa4SK6WxWXS)
![The "Forgetting Curve" of eyewitness identification is an application of absolute threshold and signal detection theory. The original uploader was Icez at English Wikipedia. [Public domain], via Wikimedia Commons rssalemscience-20180712-2-171809.jpg](https://imageserver.ebscohost.com/img/embimages/ers/sp/embedded/rssalemscience-20180712-2-171809.jpg?ephost1=dGJyMNHX8kSepq84xNvgOLCmsE2epq5Srqa4SK6WxWXS)
Background
The term absolute threshold was first used in the 1890s by psychologists experimenting to determine the least level of stimuli that a person could detect. The concept originated in the work of German physician Ernst Heinrich Weber and German experimental psychologist Gustav Fechner. Weber, who is considered the father of experimental psychology, developed the concept of measuring levels of things that are perceived and determining the relationships between them. Fechner studied sensory perception and was interested in determining the full range of a type of sensation—for instance, the lowest and highest pitch of a musical note—that people could perceive.
Fechner developed the idea of the absolute threshold as the lowest level. He and Weber worked with a unit called a just noticeable difference (JND) in measuring levels of stimuli. Fechner also recognized that there could be levels of a stimulus that a person does not consciously register, but that creates a subconscious reaction. He did not include these in his assessment of absolute threshold. Instead, he maintained that the absolute threshold was the lowest level of stimuli that created conscious recognition in an organism.
It is important to note that absolute threshold is related to but not the same as the difference threshold. The difference threshold is the least amount of variation between two similar stimuli that can be detected, such as degrees of temperature. This concept was also part of the work of Weber and Fechner. However, while absolute threshold is always related to the least amount of a stimulus that can be noticed, the difference threshold can be determined between two variations of a stimulus at any level of detection. For example, researchers helping to design traffic signals might investigate the difference threshold between two shades of red to decide which is easiest to see by the most people.
Overview
Researchers use one of several methods to determine absolute threshold. The method used depends in part on the type of stimuli being measured, other variables that could be factors, and the intended use of the results of the study. There are three classic methods used in psychophysical research, and one newer variation that is used most often in contemporary study.
The three classical methods are the adjustment method, the constant stimuli method, and the limits method. The newer method is called the staircase method. Researchers choose a method that best suits the type of research being conducted.
In the adjustment method, the study participant is asked to bring the stimulus to a level where the participant can detect it. This might involve something like turning a knob to increase volume of a sound until it can be heard. It can also be used in reverse, turning the sound down until it can no longer be heard. The process is generally repeated multiple times, and the researcher uses an average of the results to determine the actual absolute threshold.
The method of limits and the constant stimuli method are similar. In each, the researcher chooses a number of specific levels of stimuli, such as the same sound at a number of different volumes. The levels are deliberately chosen to be below, at, and above the anticipated absolute threshold. In the limits method, the stimuli are presented in order from least to greatest or vice versa. In the constant stimuli method, the stimuli are presented randomly. In either method, the test participant is asked to indicate when the stimulus is detected. As with the adjustment method, the test is repeated and the results combined to allow for outside influences on the test process.
The staircase method was developed by American experimental psychologist Tom Norman Cornsweet in 1962. This method starts with a few variations of a stimulus and then adjusts them up or down depending on the study participant’s perception. For example, a tone will be played, and if the participant indicates it is heard, the sound will be adjusted downward until it can no longer be heard. Conversely, if it is not heard, the sound will be adjusted upward in increments until it is detected.
As with all the other methods, a researcher using the staircase method will repeat the study several times with the same participant and use a combination of the results to determine the absolute threshold. This allows for the many factors that can affect a person’s perception of a stimulus, including other stimuli in the test environment, the person’s overall level of health and well-being, and other elements. The researcher might also take into account such factors as the person’s age, since research has determined that the ability to detect certain levels of some stimuli changes with age.
Researchers also note that any individual’s absolute threshold can change from day to day. It can also be intentionally altered. This often happens by either continued exposure or expectation. The phenomenon sometimes called “nose blindness” is an example of this; people who are exposed to certain smells become oblivious to them, regardless of how noxious the odor might be, because the continued presence of the stimuli renders it less noticeable. People can also become more acutely aware of a stimulus and lower their absolute threshold by actively looking for clues to its presence. Parents who hear the faintest cry of their newborn in a noisy room are exhibiting the way paying attention can lower the absolute threshold.
Bibliography
“Absolute Threshold.” Athabasca University, psych.athabascau.ca/html/Glossary/demo‗glossary.cgi?mode=history&term‗id=825&color‗id=3. Accessed 28 Dec. 2018.
“Absolute Thresholds.” University of Calgary, www.ucalgary.ca/pip369/mod1/measuring/absolute. Accessed 28 Dec. 2018.
“Basic Psychophysics.” University of Hanover, psych.hanover.edu/classes/sensation/Labs/psychophysics‗I.html. Accessed 28 Dec. 2018.
Cherry, Kendra. “Absolute Threshold: A Definition with Examples.” Explore Psychology, 3 Nov. 2023, www.explorepsychology.com/absolute-threshold/. Accessed 7 Nov. 2024.
Cherry, Kendra. “How Absolute Threshold of a Stimulus Is Used in Experimental Research.” Verywell Mind, 13 Oct. 2018, www.verywellmind.com/what-is-the-absolute-threshold-2795221. Accessed 28 Dec. 2018.
Cherry, Kendra. "What Is Absolute Threshold?" VeryWellMind, 7 Sept. 2023, www.verywellmind.com/what-is-the-absolute-threshold-2795221#:~:text=An%20absolute%20threshold%20is%20the,taste%2C%20sight%2C%20and%20smell. Accessed 7 Nov. 2024.
Cornsweet, Tom. Seeing: How Light Tells Us about the World. U of California P, 2017.
Lavalli, Tim. “The Poker Shrink: Absolute Threshold and the Signal Detection Theory.” Poker News, 9 Mar. 2007, www.pokernews.com/news/2007/03/poker-shrink-vol-31-absolute-threshold.htm. Accessed 28 Dec. 2018.
“Sensation and Perception.” Purdue University Fort Wayne, users.ipfw.edu/abbott/120/thresholds.html. Accessed 28 Dec. 2018.