Socialization in Schools
Socialization in schools is a critical process through which children learn to interact with peers, adopt social norms, and develop their identities. This process is often viewed as a lifelong journey, beginning in early childhood and evolving through adolescence. During these formative years, children typically categorize peers based on factors such as age, sex, and race, which can influence their social dynamics. Peer groups play a significant role, creating unique cultures that pass down norms and behaviors from older to younger members.
Research indicates that children often self-segregate by gender and age within school settings, though this segregation tends to diminish as they enter adolescence, when other factors like interests and social status become more prominent. While traditional schooling is commonly seen as a vital environment for socialization, the rise of homeschooling has raised concerns about the potential impacts on children's social development. Critics of homeschooling suggest that it may limit opportunities for children to engage in social learning experiences, while proponents argue that it allows for tailored educational approaches. Understanding these dynamics is essential for grasping how socialization in educational contexts shapes children's development and future interactions.
Socialization in Schools
Socialization can be defined as the type of social learning that occurs when a person interacts with others. While some believe this process is limited to childhood years, others argue socialization is a continuous process that stretches over a lifetime. Research has shown that gender group associations are the primary concern when children go from preschool age to adolescent years. Although there are many legitimate reasons why homeschooling has become popular, there is a concern about the effects of homeschooling on children, especially as it relates to the child's ability to socialize.
Keywords Adolescence; Elimination; Group Socialization Theory; Homeschooling; Peer Groups; Segregation; Selection; Social Learning; Socialization; The Social Networks Research Group
Socialization in Schools
Overview
Socialization can be defined as the type of social learning that occurs when a person interacts with others. While some believe this process is limited to childhood years, others argue socialization is a continuous process that stretches over a lifetime.
Psychologists, sociologists, and other researchers have studied socialization and social development, allowing practitioners to guide people through the socialization process. Social learning theory is especially helpful in understanding socialization and the most appropriate ways to guide a person through the process.
Selecting Peer Groups
"The first step in human group affiliation is the categorization of people into groups" (Harris, 1995, p. 466). Experts disagree on when humans develop the ability to categorize—Jean Piaget claimed that children develop this ability by the time they are toddlers, but more recent research indicates that infants can categorize people, too. Regardless of age, most people categorize others according to age, sex, and race. Young children are the exception, as most do not begin to make racial distinctions until they reach preschool age. Infants, however, are believed to recognize differences in age and gender by the time they reach one year. Interestingly, infants also demonstrate a preference for other infants, and by the time they are two years old, begin to prefer children of their own sex (Harris, 1995).
Peer Groupings & Socialization
Corsaro (1993) defines childhood socialization as the "production of and movement through a series of peer cultures" (p. 361). According to Corsaro (1993), all childhood peer groups create their own culture. And, as Harris notes, "though this series of cultures is capable of adapting to changing times, it is also capable of remaining relatively unchanged while cohort after cohort of children passes through it" (Harris, 1995, p. 470). Through these series of unique childhood peer cultures, children pass down group norms to younger children as they age and move into older peer groups. For example, a group of eight- and nine-year-olds may teach a six-year-old to play hide and seek or speak Pig Latin even as they are growing older and learning from other, older children how to play basketball or a complicated card game. In this way, groups' norms are passed down from generation to generation.
Children's Groupings
Drawing on Harris' group socialization theory, Ironstrack, Klee, McKay, and Minera (2005) write, "culture may not be transmitted from individual to individual, but from group to group" (p. 3). If Harris is correct, it can be said that children learn primarily from their own peer groups as well as from older groups—including their parents' peer groups—rather than from their parents directly. It can also be inferred that cultural transmission occurs from parents' peer groups to children's peer groups rather than from parent to child directly.
Though there may be cultural variations in parenting practices, the children's playgroup is universal (Harris, 1995). Across cultures, small playgroups will include both boys and girls and a wide range of ages; large playgroups, though, tend to divide along the lines of sex and age (Harris, 1995). And although large groups of girls tend to split up into dyads and triads, these smaller groups will usually be made up of girls who belong to similar social categories, such as age (Harris, 1995). These tendencies demonstrate that children see themselves as members of social categories, even though they may not know all the members of a category, and even if the members of a category are not all located in one place.
For example, in many cultures, a community's children are brought together to attend school. These groups of children tend to be large and composed of individuals of the same age, but that is where the similarities tend to end. Although schools generally group children together by age, children tend to choose to group themselves according to sex as well, even when school authorities disapprove of this segregation (Harris, 1995). For example, during lunchtime children will often divide themselves according to sex (Harris, 1995). The girls may sit together at one table while the boys sit together at a separate table. In schools that have a high degree of racial or ethnic diversity, children may also separate themselves according to these distinctions. However, sex tends to be the most important distinction that children make (Harris, 1995).
Once children reach adolescence, sex segregation diminishes. Instead, adolescents group themselves according to other criteria like "athletic, social or academic interests;… race, ethnicity, social class; and… proclivities such as drug use and delinquency” (Harris, 1995). According to Harris (1995), "two changes occur between early and mid-adolescence: gender ceases to be the primary indicator of group identity, and size ceases to be a useful indication of age and status" (p. 471). Adolescents who live in societies that do not confer adulthood upon them as soon as they reach physical maturation tend to categorize themselves as belonging to an adolescent group that is distinct from adult groups. Harris (1995) uses this tendency to argue that much of adolescent behavior results from adolescents' desire to distinguish themselves from adults, rather than from their aspiration to become adults, as other researchers have argued. To make this distinction, Harris (1995) says, adolescents may dress, speak, and behave differently from adults.
However, others disagree with Harris. Moffitt (2003) asserts that "adolescent delinquency must be a social behavior that allows access to some desirable resource" and suggests that "the 'resource' is mature status, with its consequent power and privilege" (p. 686).
Application
Children's Peer Groups in Schools
The Social Networks research group, composed of faculty and students from the Department of Psychology at Portland State University, has focused its research on children's peer networks. Its members are primarily concerned with how peer group processes can "promote or undermine intra-individual change in a child's academic development" (Sage, Hillier, Weaver, Newton-Curtis, & Kindermann, 2002, p. 3). One facet of the group's research has been how children join, leave, and exclude other children from their groups, as well as how children's groups impact their group members. The researchers have identified three categories of processes:
• Selection: The "expression of children's associative preferences through the bids they make to join certain peer groups, and through their attempts, once they belong to a group, to maintain existing or recruit additional members."
• Elimination: A process "in which children attempt to exclude others, or actually leave a group themselves."
• Socialization: A process "by which peer groups influence individuals" (Sage et al., 2002, p. 2).
The research group conducted a study that examined peer group processes in four mixed fourth and fifth grade classrooms in a suburban elementary school. The research team wanted to observe and evaluate the relationships between a child's school motivation or engagement, processes of selection of peer group members, and socialization influences from group members.
The 112 children and 4 teachers participating in the study were asked to complete questionnaires and engage in a series of interviews over the course of a school year to glean information about how children's peer groups relate to their academic motivation. The findings revealed that children tended to associate with other children of similar levels of motivation, and that the level of a group's motivation was maintained by adding new members and excluding members whose level of motivation had changed. Further analysis showed that individual children's levels of motivation changed relative to the level of motivation within their original peer groups.
Viewpoint
Homeschooling
According to the National Center for Education Statistics (NCES), there has been a steady rise in the number of homeschooled children in the US in the twenty-first century. In 2003, 1.1 million students were homeschooled, compared to an estimated 1.5 million in 2007, and 3.7 million in 2021. Additionally, 5.22 percent of the school-age population was homeschooled in 2022. Why is there such growth? According to Mayberry (1991), more and more families, perceiving the public schools as battlegrounds for the political and social interests of a wide variety of social sectors, are becoming disillusioned "with the quality and content of state-sponsored education" (p. 2). She argues that "the decision to homeschool…represents a political response by people who perceive a threat in the current organization and content of public education" (1991, quoted in Aiex, 1994).
There are many reasons why parents choose to homeschool. According to Mayberry (1991), three of the most popular reasons include:
• The family lives in a rural area that does not have a school nearby.
• The parents are concerned about school violence.
• The parents feel they can provide a better education than the public school system.
According to the National Center for Education Statistics, parents reported anxiety regarding the environment of schools, "such as safety, drugs, or negative peer pressure" (2022), as the biggest reason for parents to choose to homeschool their children. Other factors included the desire to provide religious instruction and dissatisfaction with the academic instruction at other schools. These results were consistent with a similar survey taken in the early 2010s.
Mayberry (1991) also found that an ideology can also prod parents to homeschool their children. Of the 1600 Oregon homeschooling families Mayberry (1991) studied, groups she identified as "deeply religious" parents and "New Age" parents emerged as the predominate groups that homeschooled children for ideological reasons. These groups' responses led her to conclude that they perceived homeschooling as a means to control what and how their children learn and, thereby, to pass their beliefs and values onto their children.
Although there are many legitimate reasons for homeschooling's popularity, there is a concern about the effects homeschooling has on children, especially as it relates to children's ability to socialize. Based on homeschooled children's relative isolation from the socialization that formal schooling offers, one may conclude that homeschooled children do not develop the skills they need to interact with environments outside of their homes. Some critics argue that a homeschooled child's self-concept can suffer because of this isolation (Aiex, 1994). Other critics believe that parents who homeschool may be too protective of their children and prevent them from developing strategies for coping with the outside world or adjusting to college life. Emile Durkheim, a French sociologist, advocated public schools as venues where children could learn the collective, secular values and morality of society (Vigilant, 2013). Others defend homeschooling, saying that during early childhood, a degree of protection is needed to foster growth (Aiex, 1994).
Terms & Concepts
Adolescence: The period of physical and psychological development from the onset of puberty to maturity.
Elimination: Processes through which children exclude others or remove themselves from a group.
Group Socialization Theory: Theory coined by Judith Harris based on the concept that, although the influence of parents on children is great, the influence of peers has a greater impact on a child's development.
Homeschooling: An alternative form of education in which parents or guardians assume responsibility for their children's education without sending them to any type of formal school.
Peer Groups: Groups made up of people who share common social characteristics such as age, class, occupation, or education, and interact on a level of equality.
Segregation: The policy or practice of separating people of different races, classes, ethnic groups, religious groups, or genders, often in a way that puts the minority group at a disadvantage.
Selection: The expression of children's associative preferences through the bids they make to join certain peer groups, and through their attempts, once they belong to a group, to maintain existing or recruit additional members (Sage, et al., 2002).
Social Learning: A change in behavior controlled by environmental influences rather than innate or internal forces.
Socialization: The process whereby a child learns the norms, values, and behaviors of a group, largely through imitation and group pressure.
The Social Network Research Group: A group comprising faculty and students from the Department of Psychology at Portland State University dedicated to studying social networks.
Bibliography
Aiex, N. (1994). Home schooling and socialization of children. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. ED372460). Retrieved March 11, 2008, from http://www.ericdigests.org/1995-1/home.htm
Brooks, J., & Lewis, M. (1976). Infants' responses to strangers: Midget, adult, and child. Child Development, 47(2), 323–332. https://doi.org/10.2307/1128785
Cairns, R. B., Perrin, J. E., & Cairns, B. D. (1985). Social structure and social cognition in early adolescence: Affiliative patterns. Journal of Early Adolescence, 5(3), 339–355. https://doi.org/10.1177/0272431685053007
Corsaro, W. (1993). Interpretive reproduction in the "scuola materna". European Journal of Psychology of Education, 8, 357–374. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03172694
Diamond, K., LeFurgy, W., & Blass, S. (1993). Attitudes of preschool children toward peers with disabilities: A year-long investigation in integrated classrooms. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 154(2), 215–221. https://doi.org/10.1080/00221325.1993.9914735
Eckert, P. (2006). Jocks and burnouts: Social categories and identity in the high school (8th ed.). Teachers College Press.
Edwards, C. (1992). Cross-cultural perspectives on family-peer relations. In R. D. Parke & G. W. Ladd (Eds.), Family peer relationships: Modes of linkage (pp. 285–316). Erlbaum.
Eimas, P., & Quinn, P. (1994). Studies on the formation of perceptually based basic- level categories in young infants. Child Development, 65(3), 903–917. https://doi.org/10.1111/J.1467-8624.1994.TB00792.X
Fagot, B. (1985). Beyond the reinforcement principle: Another step toward understanding sex role development. Developmental Psychology, 21(6), 1097–1104. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.21.6.1097
Fiske, S. (1992). Thinking is for doing: Portraits of social cognition from daguerreotype to laserphoto. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 877–889. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.63.6.877
Harris, J. (1995). Where is the child's environment? A group socialization theory of development. Psychological Review, 102, 458-489. Retrieved March 11, 2008, from http://www.apa.org/journals/features/rev1023458.pdf
Hirschfeld, L. (1993). Discovering social difference: The role of appearance in the development of racial awareness. Cognitive Psychology, 25(3), 317–350. http://hdl.handle.net/2027.42/30711
Ironstrack, T., Klee, A., McKay, S., & Minera, J. (2005, Summer). How do peer groups affect learning. Retrieved March 10, 2008, from http://www.users.muohio.edu/shermalw/EDP603SM2005/EDP60sm05-group-paper.doc
Kindermann, T. (1993). Natural peer groups as contexts for individual development: The case of children's motivation in school. Developmental Psychology, 29(6), 970–977. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.29.6.970
LaFromboise, T., Coleman, H., & Gerton, J. (1993). Psychological impact of biculturalism: Evidence and theory. Psychological Bulletin, 114(3), 395–412. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.114.3.395
Leinbach, M., & Fagot, B. (1993). Categorical habituation to male and female faces: Gender schematic processing in infancy. Infant Behavior and Development, 16, 317–332. https://doi.org/10.1016/0163-6383(93)80038-a
Maccoby, E. (1990). Gender and relationships: A developmental account. American Psychologist, 45(4), 513–520. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.45.4.513
Maccoby, E., & Jacklin, C. (1987). Gender segregation in childhood. Advances in Child Development and Behavior, 20, 239-287. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0065-2407(08)60404-8
Mandler, J. (1992). How to build a baby: II. Conceptual primitives. Psychological Review, 99(4), 587–604. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.99.4.587
Mayberry, M. (1991). Conflict and social determinism: The reprivatization of education. Paper presented at the American Educational Research Association Meeting. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. ED330107). Retrieved March 11, 2008, from http://www.eric.ed.gov/ERICWebPortal/custom/portlets/recordDetails/detailmini.jsp?_nfpb=true&_&ERICExtSearch_SearchValue_0=ED330107&ERICExtSearch_SearchType_0=no&accno=ED330107
Moffitt, T. E. (2003). Life-course-persistent and adolescence-limited antisocial behavior: A 10-year research review and a research agenda. In B. B. Lahey, T. E. Moffitt, & A. Caspi (Eds.), Causes of conduct disorder and juvenile delinquency (pp. 49–75). The Guilford Press.
National Center for Education Statistics. (2022). Homeschooled Children and Reasons for Homeschooling. Condition of Education. U.S. Department of Education, Institute of Education Sciences. Retrieved 3 June 2023, from https://nces.ed.gov/programs/coe/indicator/tgk.
Parent and family involvement in education, from the national household education surveys program of 2012. (2013). Retrieved January 9, 2015, from http://nces.ed.gov/pubs2013/2013028.pdf
Piaget, J. (1952). The origins of intelligence in children. International Universities Press.
Sadker, M., & Sadker, D. (2014). Failing at fairness: How America's schools cheat girls. Scribner's.
Sage, N., Hillier, S., Weaver, S., Newton-Curtis, L., & Kindermann, T. (2002). The role of peer group selection and socialization for children's school motivation: A replication and extension. Retrieved March 11, 2007, from http://www.sonet.pdx.edu/Publications/ISSBD-03c.pdf
Sage, N., & Kindermann, T. (1999). Peer networks, behavioral contingencies, and children's engagement in the classroom. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 45(1), 143–171.
Savin-Williams, R. (1979). An ethological study of dominance formation and maintenance in a group of human adolescents. Child Development, 49, 534–536. http://doi.org/10.1111/J.1467-8624.1976.TB02277.X
Taylor, J. (1987). Self-concept in home schooling children (1986). Dissertation. 726. https://dx.doi.org/10.32597/dissertations/726
Turner, J. (1987). Rediscovering the social group: A self-categorization theory. Basil Blackwell.
Whiting, B., & Edwards, C. (1988). Children of different worlds: The formation of social behavior. Harvard University Press.
Suggested Reading
Achhpal, B., Goldman, J., & Rohner, R. (2007). A comparison of European American and Puerto Rican parents' goals and expectations about the socialization and education of pre-school children. International Journal of Early Years Education, 15, 1–13. Retrieved March 11, 2008, from EBSCO Online Database Academic Search Premier. http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=aph&AN=25084380&site=ehost-live
Dunsmore, K., & Lagos, T. (2008). Politics, media and youth: understanding political socialization via video production in secondary schools. Learning, Media, & Technology, 33, 1–10. Retrieved March 11, 2008, from EBSCO Online Database Academic Search Premier. http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=aph&AN=30075896&site=ehost-live
Pahlke, E., Bigler, R. S., & Suizzo, M. (2012). Relations between colorblind socialization and children's racial bias: Evidence from European American mothers and their preschool children. Child Development, 83, 1164–1179. Retrieved November 1, 2013, from EBSCO Online Database SocINDEX with Full Text. http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=sih&AN=77729020
Schwartz, D., Kelly, B., & Duong, M. (2013). Do academically-engaged adolescents experience social sanctions from the peer group? Journal of Youth & Adolescence, 42, 1319–1330. Retrieved January 9, 2015, from EBSCO Online Database SocINDEX with Full Text. http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=sih&AN=89702155&site=ehost-live&scope=site
Vigilant, L., Trefethren, L., & Anderson, T. C. (2013). “You Can’t Rely on Somebody Else to Teach Them Something They Don’t Believe:” Impressions of legitimation crisis and socialization control in the narratives of Christian homeschooling fathers. Humanity & Society, 37, 201–224. Retrieved November 1, 2013, from EBSCO Online Database SocINDEX with Full Text. http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=sih&AN=90085725