Color blindness and genetics
Color blindness is a visual impairment affecting color perception, primarily linked to the function of light-sensitive cells in the retina called cones. Individuals with normal vision, known as trichromats, have three types of cones sensitive to long, medium, and short wavelengths of light, allowing them to perceive a full spectrum of colors. In contrast, color blind individuals, or dichromats, typically possess only two types of cones, leading to diminished color discrimination. Genetics plays a crucial role in color blindness; red-green color blindness is often inherited through the X chromosome, with males being more frequently affected due to their single X chromosome. Other types, like blue-yellow color blindness, arise from mutations on an autosome and are inherited in an autosomal dominant manner, affecting both genders equally. Diagnostic tests, such as the Ishihara color test, can identify color deficiencies, although no cure exists at present. Individuals with color blindness may benefit from specialized lenses or software designed to enhance color differentiation, and ongoing advancements in genetic technology may eventually provide more definitive solutions.
Color blindness and genetics
Definition Color blindness is a condition in people whose eyes lack one or more of the three color receptors present in most human eyes. It is an important condition to understand because so many people experience it to some degree. It is also a window into the inner workings of the eye and a marvelous example of the workings of Mendelian genetics.
Risk Factors
Approximately 1.2 percent of males and 0.02 percent of females are protanopes (lack L cones); 1.5 percent of males and 0.01 percent of females are deuteranopes (lack M cones); but only 0.001 percent of males and females are tritanopes (lack S cones).
![Inheritance of color blindness through the male See page for author [Public domain], via Wikimedia Commons 94416422-89125.jpg](https://imageserver.ebscohost.com/img/embimages/ers/sp/embedded/94416422-89125.jpg?ephost1=dGJyMNHX8kSepq84xNvgOLCmsE2epq5Srqa4SK6WxWXS)
![A comparison of the visible color spectrum in common types of color blindness. By Nanobot (Own work) [Public domain], via Wikimedia Commons 94416422-89126.jpg](https://imageserver.ebscohost.com/img/embimages/ers/sp/embedded/94416422-89126.jpg?ephost1=dGJyMNHX8kSepq84xNvgOLCmsE2epq5Srqa4SK6WxWXS)
Etiology and Genetics
Light-sensitive structures in the retina called cones are the basis for color vision. A person with normal vision can distinguish seven pure hues (colors) in the rainbow: violet, blue, cyan, green, yellow, orange, and red. People with normal vision are trichromats, meaning that they have three types of cones: L, M, and S, named for particular sensitivities to light of long, medium, and short wavelengths. The human vision system detects color by comparing the relative rates at which the L, M, and S cones react to light. For example, yellow light causes the M and L cones to signal at about the same rate, and the person “sees” yellow. Strangely, the right amounts of green and red stimulate these cones in the same fashion, and the person will again see the color yellow even though there is no yellow light present. Since people have only three types of color receptors, it takes the proper mix of intensities of only three primary colors to cause a person to “see” all the colors of the rainbow. A tiny droplet of water on the screen of a color television or computer monitor will act like a magnifying lens and reveal that the myriad colors that are displayed are formed from tiny dots of only blue, green, and red.
People are referred to as “color blind” if they are dichromats, that is, if they have only two of the three types of cones. Tritanopes cannot distinguish between blue (especially greenish shades) and yellow. The genetic code for the S pigment lies on chromosome 7. The fact that the S pigment gene lies on an autosome explains why yellow-blue color blindness is manifested equally in males and females. The inheritance pattern is that of an autosomal dominant trait: Only one arm of the two arms of chromosome 7 has the defective allele in the affected parent, and since there is a 50 percent chance a child will receive the defective arm, 50 percent of the children will inherit the defect. In fact, the trait is often incompletely expressed, so that the majority of affected individuals retain some reduced S-cone function.
Anomalous trichromats are more common than dichromats. They need three primary colors to match the hues of the rainbow, but they match them with different intensities than normal trichromats do because the peak sensitivities of their cones occur at wavelengths slightly different from normal. Their color confusion is similar to that of the dichromats, but less severe. About 1 percent of males and 0.03 percent of females have anomalous L cones, while 4.5 percent of males and 0.4 percent of females have anomalous M cones. The fact that far more males than females have some degree of red-green color blindness implies that the genetic information for the pigments in L and M cones lies on the X chromosome.
The gene structures for M-cone and L-cone pigments are 96 percent the same, so it is likely that one began as a mutation of the other. Small mutations in either gene can slightly shift the color of peak absorption in the cones and produce an anomalous trichromat. Generally these mutations make M and L cones more alike. The similarity between the genes and the fact that they are adjacent to each other on the X chromosome can lead to a variety of copying errors during meiosis. People with normal color vision have one L-cone gene and one to three M-cone genes. The complete omission of either type of gene will result in severe red-green color blindness: protanopia or deuteranopia. Hybrid genes that are a combination of L-cone and M-cone genes lead to less severe types of red-green color blindness, especially if there is also a normal copy of the gene present.
Red-green color blindness follows an X-gene recessive inheritance pattern. Suppose that a man has a defective X gene (and is therefore color blind) and a woman is normal. Their male children are normal because they inherited their X genes from their mother, but their female children will be carriers because they had to inherit one X gene from their father. If the daughters married normal men, 50 percent of the grandsons got the defective gene from their mothers and were color blind, and 50 percent of the grandsons were normal. Likewise, 50 percent of the granddaughters were normal and 50 percent inherited the defective gene from their mothers and became carriers.
Symptoms
Dichromats can match all of the colors they see in the rainbow by mixing only two primary colors of light, but they see fewer (and different) hues in the rainbow than a person with normal vision. Protanopes and deuteranopes cannot distinguish between red and green. More exactly, protanopes tend to confuse reds, grays, and bluish blue-greens, while deuteranopes tend to confuse purples, grays, and greenish blue-greens.
Screening and Diagnosis
The test most often used to diagnose red-green color blindness is called the Ishihara color test. It consists of a series of pictures of colored spots in which a figure (such as a number or symbol) is embedded using in a slightly different color. Those with normal color vision can easily distinguish the figure within the image, but those with color deficiencies cannot.
Treatment and Therapy
While color blindness cannot be cured, devices such as tinted filters or contact lenses can help an individual distinguish between different colors. Their practical use is somewhat limited, however. Computer software and cybernetic devices can also aid those born with this condition. Gene technology may soon be able to cure color blindness.
Prevention and Outcomes
Color blindness cannot be prevented, but those born with it may manage so well that they may be unaware of the condition if not tested for it.
Bibliography
Beck, Melinda. “New Outlook on Colorblindness.” Wall Street Journal—Eastern Edition 6 Nov. 2012: D1+. OmniFile Full Text Mega (H.W. Wilson). Web. 23 July 2014.
Besharse, Joseph, and Dean Bok. The Retina and Its Disorders. Burlington: Elsevier Science, 2011. Digital file.
Hsia, Yun, and C. H. Graham. “Color Blindness.” The Science of Color. Ed. Alex Byrne and David R. Hilbert. Cambridge: MIT, 1997. Print.
Katsnelson, Alla. "Colour Me Better: Figures for Colour Blindness." Nature, 4 Oct. 2021, www.nature.com/articles/d41586-021-02696-z. Accessed 2 Nov. 2022.
Mamalis, George. “Color Blindness.” CRS—Pediatric Advisor (2013): 1. Consumer Health Complete—EBSCOhost. Web. 23 July 2014.
McIntyre, D. A. Colour Blindness: Causes and Effects. Chester: Dalton, 2002. Print.
Medeiros, John A. Cone Shape and Color Vision: Unification of Structure and Perception. Blountsville: Fifth Estate, 2006. Print.
Nathans, Jeremy. “The Genes for Color Vision.” The Science of Color. Ed. by Alex Byrne and David R. Hilbert. Cambridge: MIT, 1997. Print.
Neitz, Jay, and Maureen Neitz. “The Genetics of Normal and Defective Color Vision.” Vision Research 51.7 (2011): 633–51. Ergonomics Abstracts. Web. 23 July 2014.
Rosenthal, Odeda, and Robert H. Phillips. Coping with Color Blindness. Garden City Park: Avery, 1997. Print.
Traboulsi, Elias I. Genetic Diseases of the Eye. New York: Oxford UP, 2012. Print.
Wagner, Robert P. “Understanding Inheritance: An Introduction to Classical and Molecular Genetics.” The Human Genome Project: Deciphering the Blueprint of Heredity. Ed. Necia Grant Cooper. Mill Valley: University Science Books, 1994. Print.