Pattern vision and perception
Pattern vision and perception refer to the human visual system's ability to detect and interpret visual patterns through a complex series of processes. The visual system uses light primarily as a medium to reveal patterns, discarding absolute light intensities and instead focusing on relative intensities and colors. This processing begins at the retina, where receptor cells gather information, and continues through various stages, ultimately reaching the visual cortex. Here, neurons respond to specific patterns, such as lines and their orientations, which are crucial for recognizing shapes and objects.
Beyond basic pattern recognition, the system incorporates additional cues such as motion and shading to resolve ambiguities, enabling us to interpret the structure and depth of objects. For instance, motion helps distinguish between different shapes, while shading offers insights into an object's orientation concerning light sources. Understanding pattern vision is increasingly important in fields like artificial intelligence and virtual reality, where replicating human-like pattern recognition in machines is a significant challenge. Researchers study various levels of analysis—neurophysiological, algorithmic, and behavioral—to unravel how visual information is processed and understood, revealing the intricate mechanisms that allow for rapid and accurate interpretation of the visual world.
Pattern vision and perception
- TYPE OF PSYCHOLOGY: Sensation and perception
- Human pattern vision and machine pattern vision have many properties in common. After extracting lines and other elements from a visual scene, pattern vision devices extract depth using many kinds of cues, and identify objects by interactions with memory. The knowledge can be applied in such settings as virtual reality.
Introduction
The human visual system is designed to see patterns. In fact, the system uses light only as a medium for detecting patterns; the actual intensities of light in various parts of the visual field are discarded at the first stage of visual processing, and only relative intensities remain. The fate of these patterns of intensities in the visual brain is the concern of pattern vision.


Vision relies on two kinds of information: patterns of intensity and patterns of color. Intensity patterns are resolved into objects in a number of steps. The steps can be conceived either in terms of the responses of single neurons at various levels of the visual pathways or as a series of rules, or algorithms, governing the transformations that visual information undergoes. The process begins in the retinas of the two eyes, where each receptor cell is exposed to a tiny sample of the visual world, a small spot of sensitivity. The receptor cells pass their messages to other cells at higher levels, and the visual image is reorganized, or recoded, at each step.
Vision Processing
From the point of view of pattern vision, a particularly significant step occurs when the signals enter the visual cortex, after several stages of processing. Each neuron in the cortex is excited by signals from many receptors lying in a straight line, so that the neuron responds best to a line in the visual field. The line is a receptive field, an area of the world to which the neuron responds. The line must have a particular location and orientation. The neuron responds best, in fact, to a group of parallel lines at a particular spacing. This helps to improve the reliability of the system. Other neurons, at the same time, respond to lines at other locations, orientations, and spacings.
From this set of line-shaped receptive fields, the visual system constructs an internal model of the visual world. First, the outlines of objects can be recognized from patterns of lines. Next, particular patterns of lines are assigned particular meanings. For example, sometimes two lines will meet at a T junction. In this case, the visual system generally assumes that the crossbar of the T is in front of the upright part because the crossbar interrupts a line. From an image that contains many such lines and intersections, the visual system reconstructs the surfaces and objects of the world.
Some modern research has indicated that previous visual perceptions influence human vision. This phenomenon, called serial dependence, occurs when one’s perception of current stimuli is impacted by historical visual stimuli or stimuli seen just moments before. The brain stores information about visual experiences and uses those past events to inform later patterns of vision and perception.
Motion and Shading
Often, however, this is not enough, and ambiguities or unanswered questions may remain about what objects are present in the world. The visual system has several additional methods by which to interpret the image. One is motion, which is usually present in the visual field. If there is no motion of objects in the world, there is often motion of the observer, so objects move past the observer at different rates. The visual system can take any group of lines moving in the same way and conclude that they represent a single object moving relative to other parts of the visual world.
Objects can also reveal their shapes by their motion. A circular image, for example, might be either a disk or a sphere. A rotating sphere, however, will not change its shape, while a flipping disk will appear round, then elliptical, then flat as it tumbles. Other objects have characteristic changes in appearance as they rotate, providing the visual system with information about their structure. How the system decodes this information is the shape-from-motion problem.
The shading of objects also reveals something about their structure. A surface that is facing a source of illumination will be brighter than a surface at another angle. A rounded object will have continuous changes in shading. This is the shape-from-shading problem, and solving it gives still more information about the structure of the visual world. In this way, the visual system combines information from many cues, or sources of input from the image, and normally, the brain does a remarkably good job of interpreting the visual world quickly and accurately.
Color gives still more information about visual patterns, helping the system distinguish surfaces and textures. It is handled by separate visual mechanisms and cannot resolve as much detail as the form-processing mechanism.
Machine Vision
One reason it is important to know how pattern vision works is that an understanding of visual processing is necessary to enable machines to interact effectively with the visual environment. As humans interact more with patterns generated in graphics-oriented computers and in art, it becomes important to understand what goes on in the human mind when patterns are processed. One necessity for building robots to do many tasks is to give them the ability to recognize objects in their surroundings. Generally, the robots are computer-based and use television cameras for visual input; interpretation of the image comes next. This has proved to be more difficult than anticipated; even the first step in the process, abstracting lines and edges from the world, remains imperfect in existing systems. One of the efforts in the area of artificial pattern recognition, then, has been to investigate the pattern-recognition mechanisms of humans and animals and to try to build similar mechanisms into the machines. Every three-year-old has far better pattern-recognition capabilities than the most sophisticated machines.
One of the problems facing machine vision is that, although the identification of lines, edges, and patterns might work well in the laboratory, the process is less successful in the real world. Humans have a remarkable ability to recognize patterns even in “noisy” environments, despite shadows, occlusions, changes in perspective, and other sources of variation. The discipline of artificial intelligence is concerned with such problems.
Virtual Reality
Another application of visual pattern recognition is in the area of virtual reality, the effort to design displays that create in a computer user the illusion that one is actually in a different environment. Usually, the observer wears goggles that present images to the two eyes, reproducing everything that the observer would see in another environment. When the user’s head turns, the environment presented in the goggles rotates in the opposite way, just as it would in the real world. The system might present an undersea or space environment, for example, and can even include an artificial image of the observer’s own hand calculated from the position of an electronically equipped glove worn by the user. Here, the difficulty is in deciding what the display should offer to the user; it is impractical to reproduce an entire visual world in all its richness and detail. The designer of a virtual-reality system must select the patterns that will be presented and must know what information is essential to pattern vision and what can be left out. Again, knowledge of what information the human visual system will extract from the scene is essential to guide decisions about what to present.
An example of how research in pattern vision can influence the design of virtual-reality systems is in the amount of detail that must be presented to the observer. The fovea of the eye, at the center of vision, sees much finer detail than the rest of the retina, and the farther from the fovea one goes, the less detail can be resolved. Engineers take advantage of this property of human vision by designing systems that present rich detail near objects of interest and less detail elsewhere. It is easier to update the information in this kind of display than to recalculate a finely detailed image for the whole visual field. Similarly, color information need not be presented in great detail over the entire visual field.
Other economies in design can be used in virtual-reality systems as well as in other computer displays. One such shortcut takes advantage of more subtle properties of visual pattern processing. The shading cues that are used to give an object the appearance of depth need not be accurate ones. The visual system is not sensitive to some kinds of distortions in shading and will accept an object as appropriately shaded even if the mathematics that generate the computer’s shading are simplified and distorted.
Another example of a simplification that engineers can make is in the presentation of motion. Humans are not very sensitive to differences in rates of acceleration, so these differences need not be presented accurately. In summary, the human visual system uses shortcuts in interpreting the visual image, and artificial systems can use similar shortcuts in constructing the image.
Theoretical Developments
The beginnings of research on pattern vision can be traced to the work of René Descartes in the seventeenth century. Descartes dissected a cow’s eye and found that a small upside-down image of the world was projected on the back of the eye. All the information that comes from vision passes through a similar stage in human eyes as well. For more than two centuries, however, little progress was made in deciphering what happened to visual information after it left the retina. Anatomists learned where in the brain the visual fibers led, but they could not find out what was happening there.
One of the advances that has made work on pattern vision possible is the realization that vision must be studied at many levels of analysis. One level is neurophysiology, the understanding of what goes on in the nerve cells and in the fibers that connect them. Another level is the algorithm, the set of internal rules for coding and interpreting visual information. Researchers at this level ask what steps the visual system must take to interpret a pattern. The steps themselves are taken care of at the neurophysiological level. A third level is behavior: Researchers investigate the capabilities of pattern vision in the intact human. At this more global level, one studies visual pattern processing as a whole rather than dissecting its pieces. It is relating one level to another that advances understanding. At the behavior level, it is observed that people are capable of recognizing patterns from lines alone, as in cartoons. At the algorithmic level, it is found that extracting lines from an image is a useful step in interpreting the image. At the neurophysiological level, it is found that some neurons are sensitive to lines in the visual world.
Modern theories of pattern vision all share several ideas. First, information is transformed in small steps, not all at once, from the image to its meaning. The early steps are largely independent of the use to be made of the visual pattern. Later steps involve interactions with memory and with the use to be made of the visual information. At these later stages, even single nerve cells code information from a wide region of the visual field, as these cells have the job of integrating images from large areas. At the algorithmic level, the brain engages a number of assumptions about the structure of the visual world and the objects in it to interpret a scene quickly and reliably. The visual field is represented over and over in the brain as information passes to more specialized regions that emphasize movement, pattern recognition, visual-motor interactions, and other uses that the brain makes of visual inputs.
Another common idea in pattern vision is that the image is analyzed in several different ways at once. Depth, for example, might be sought in stereoscopic vision (small differences in the images arriving at the two eyes), in superpositions (using the T junctions described above, among other methods), in shading, and in other clues. If one method does not come up with a meaningful interpretation, another one will. In this way, a reliable pattern vision system can be created from unreliable components.
Bibliography
Bridgeman, Bruce. The Biology of Behavior and Mind. Wiley, 1988.
Davis, Joel. “Brain and Visual Perception: The Story of a 25-Year Collaboration.” Color Research & Application, vol. 31, no. 2, 2006, p. 156, doi.org/10.1002/col.20195. Accessed 10 Dec. 2024.
Daw, Nigel W. Visual Development. 3rd ed., Springer, 2014.
Del Viva, Maria M., et al. “Information and Perception of Meaningful Patterns.” PLoS ONE, vol. 8, no. 7, 2013, doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0069154. Accessed 10 Dec. 2024.
Gregory, Richard L. Eye and Brain: The Psychology of Seeing. 5th ed., Princeton UP, 1997.
Harris, John, and Jared Smith. Sensation and Perception. 2nd ed., SAGE Publications Ltd., 2022.
Humphreys, Glyn W., and M. Jane Riddoch. To See but Not to See: A Case Study of Visual Agnosia. 1987. Erlbaum, 1995.
Luo, Junlian, and Thérèse Collins. “The Representational Similarity between Visual Perception and Recent Perceptual History.” The Journal of Neuroscience, vol. 43, no. 20, 2023, pp. 3658–65, doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2068-22.2023. Accessed 10 Dec. 2024.
Remington, Lee Ann, and Denise Goodwin. Clinical Anatomy and Physiology of the Visual System. 4th ed., Elsevier, 2022.
Strasburger, Hans, et al. “Peripheral Vision and Pattern Recognition: A Review.” Journal of Vision, vol. 11, no. 5, 2011, p. 13, doi.org/10.1167/11.5.13. Accessed 10 Dec. 2024.
Tovée, Martin J. An Introduction to the Visual System. 2nd ed., Cambridge UP, 2008.
Waltz, David L. “Artificial Intelligence.” Scientific American, Oct. 1982, pp. 118–33.