Prosopagnosia

A brain disorder that renders people unable to differentiate among human faces, prosopagnosia (also known as face blindness) is one of a cluster of cognitive impairment syndromes caused by damage to nerve cells in the brain area responsible for recognizing shapes and textures. These syndromes may take different paths, leaving its sufferers unable to differentiate between living beings or between human-made objects.

89550633-58377.jpg

Prosopagnosia and related processing disorders may be congenital or acquired (as a result of cerebral trauma, for instance). Face blindness may appear in different forms, patterns, and levels of recognition, such as the inability to recognize individuals by the face, impaired learning of new faces, or delusional misidentification, among others.

Overview

Cases of face blindness and misrecognition were first documented in the nineteenth century. It was not until the 1940s, however, that Joachim Bodamer identified these disorders by the term prosopagnosia. Research in face recognition disorders has revealed a number of important elements related to the context of human face processing. The involvement of a few specific brain regions was first detected in the 1950s and confirmed by several follow-up studies in the 1960s and 1970s. Individuals with prosopagnosia may suffer from an inability to learn new faces or from an inability to recognize once-familiar faces. In cases of brain injury, for example, patients may recognize faces from before their injury, but are unable to learn new faces, and in some cases they may show impaired processing of visual information. In most cases, sufferers recognize facial expressions in general but have difficulty recognizing the facial features of individuals. Although there is a wide range of capacity levels in face recognition, individuals affected with prosopagnosia commonly show no impairment of general memory. They can recognize names and perform aptly in tests of verbal memory. In laboratory studies, patients recognize familiar names even though they may not be able to match them to their respective faces.

An extreme form of prosopagnosia is a delusional misidentification disorder known as Capgrass syndrome (or Capgrass delusion), identified by Joseph Capgrass and Jean Reboul-Lachaux in the 1920s. Delusional misidentification syndromes involve the misrecognition of people, objects, and places. Capgrass syndrome may occur in patients suffering focal brain damage due to trauma or disease, and it manifests when an individual develops the delusion that a friend or relative has been replaced by an identical impostor.

Although face recognition may come effortlessly to most people, it involves very complex neural computations. How most of these processes work are still beyond scientists’ knowledge, even though explanatory theories and precision in diagnosis have steadily improved since the 1980s. Studies of dissociations between intact and impaired skills due to brain lesions have contributed in important ways to the understanding of different forms of face processing and related processes of perception. This has opened the way for a wide range of treatments for prosopagnosia and related disorders. Since the 1990s, clinical tests have shown improved performance on face-matching tasks by way of development of relevant stimulus dimensions.

Bibliography

Dalrymple, Kirsten A., et al. "'A Room Full of Strangers Every Day': The Psychosocial Impact of Developmental Prosopagnosia on Children and Their Families." Journal of Psychosomatic Research 77.2 (2014): 144–50. Print.

De Gelder, Beatrice, et al. “A Modulatory Role for Facial Expressions in Prosopagnosia” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the Unites States of America 100.22 (2003): 13105–10. Print.

Lander, Karen, and Natalie Butcher. "Independence of Face Identity and Expression Processing: Exploring the Role of Motion." Frontiers in Psychology 6 (2015): 1–6. Print.

Lee, Yunio. Face Perception and Prosopagnosia: Psychophysical Investigation of Face Representation across Changes in Viewpoint and Image Size. Saarbrücken: VDM Verlag. 2007. Print.

Mindick, Nancy. Understanding Facial Recognition Difficulties in Children: Prosopagnosia Management Strategies for Parents and Professionals. London: Kingsley, 2010. Print.

Righart, Ruthger, and Beatrice de Gelder. “Impaired Face and Body Perception in Developmental Prosopagnosia.” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 104.43 (2007): 17234–38. Print.

Sacks, Oliver. “Face-blind. Why Are Some of Us Terrible at Recognizing Faces?” The New Yorker 30 Aug. 2010: 36–43. Print.

Sacks, Oliver. The Man Who Mistook His Wife for a Hat and Other Clinical Tales. New York: Simon, 1985. Print.

Sacks, Oliver. The Mind’s Eye. New York: Knopf, 2010. Print.

Sellers, Heather. You Don’t Look Like Anyone I Know. New York: Riverhead, 2011. Print.

Zhang, Jiedong, Jia Liu, and Yaoda Xu. "Neural Decoding Reveals Impaired Face Configurai Processing in the Right Fusiform Face Area of Individuals with Developmental Prosopagnosia." Journal of Neuroscience 35.4 (2015): 1539–548. Print.