Just-noticeable difference

Just-noticeable difference is defined as the smallest possible difference between two stimuli that is detectable at least half of the time. In other words, a just-noticeable difference is the difference between two similar things that is just big enough to be noticeable. Also referred to as the difference threshold, just-noticeable difference is a key concept in the scientific field of psychophysics. Psychophysics is a branch of psychology that deals with the relationships between physical stimuli and mental perception. The scientific principles of just-noticeable difference are described by Weber’s Law, a theory first developed by nineteenth-century anatomist and physiologist Ernst Weber. Weber and other scientists studied just-noticeable differences for numerous dimensions, including weight, the loudness of sounds, the brightness of lights, line length, and more. All of this research has yielded a great deal of information about sensation and the way people perceive stimuli in the world around them.

rsspencyclopedia-20190828-19-175991.jpg

Background

The scientific roots of just-noticeable difference date back to the early nineteenth century. It was then that some scientists started applying the scientific method to study how people perceive the physical environment. These researchers developed a branch of science called psychophysics. Psychophysics was the first scientific field in which experimental methods were used to study mental responses to physical stimuli. Early research in psychophysics was first carried out in the 1820s. It revolved around determining how strong a given stimulus had to be for a person to be able to detect it. This research yielded several important psychophysical terms, including threshold, below threshold, and above threshold. Stimuli, too weak to be perceived by a human subject, were referred to as below threshold. Stimuli strong enough to be perceived were called above threshold. The threshold itself, then, was the precise dividing line between stimuli that were below threshold and stimuli that were above threshold. Researchers quickly discovered that the difference threshold was not always a constant. Variable environmental conditions and other factors meant that test subjects were sometimes more or less sensitive to stimuli. As a result, the threshold was formally established as the point at which a stimulus could be detected at least half of the time. This point was called the absolute threshold.

The establishment of the absolute threshold soon led to the development of another important psychophysical concept: the difference threshold, or the just-noticeable difference. The just-noticeable difference was defined as the minimum change in a stimulus that is detectable at least half of the time. Scientists determined the just-noticeable difference by changing a stimulus by varying amounts during a test. As such a test unfolded, the researcher would continually ask the subject whether they could detect any difference in the stimulus and record the moment when the subject reported detecting a difference. One of the scientists who played a key role in this area of study was anatomist and physiologist Ernst Weber. Weber developed an equation stating that people become less sensitive to differences in signal strength as a stimulus increases. This equation became known as Weber’s Law. Put in simpler terms, Weber’s Law says that the just-noticeable difference increases in proportion to the intensity of a stimulus.

Overview

Just-noticeable difference is a component of the broader concepts of sensation and perception. Sensation is the process through which the body’s sensory organs are stimulated by physical energy from objects in the environment. Perception is how the brain interprets and organizes the information it receives through sensation. With this in mind, psychophysics is the scientific study of the relationship between the physical properties of stimuli and how people perceive those stimuli.

In psychophysics, the just-noticeable difference is said to be the smallest change is a stimulus that is detectable at least half of the time. At the most basic level, the fundamental concept of just-noticeable difference can be demonstrated through an experiment using a pair of weighted objects. If a person were asked to hold two objects of different weights, the just-noticeable difference would be the smallest weight difference between the two objects that they could detect. However, in reality, the just-noticeable difference is a bit more complex than that, mostly because it can vary based on other factors. Simply put, the human body does not respond the same way to the same stimuli on every occasion. Among other things, conditions in the surrounding environment can impact how a person perceives a given stimulus. In some cases, the intensity of a stimulus can also affect how someone might perceive a change in that stimulus. For example, a person sitting in a darkened room is likely to perceive even the slightest increase in the room’s lighting level. On the other hand, the same person would be unlikely to perceive the same increase in the lighting level of a room that is already more brightly lit. This example illustrates one of the key aspects of the just-noticeable difference: as the magnitude of a stimulus grows, a larger actual difference for detection is required. Another way to look at this principle is through the example involving weights. If a person is holding a 5-pound weight and another 5-pound weight is added to it, the person will undoubtedly perceive a difference in the overall weight of the objects. On the other hand, a person holding a 50-pound weight probably would not notice a difference if another 5-pound weight was added to the load. In order for that to happen, a heavier weight would have to be added.

Outside its scientific context, the just-noticeable difference also has applications in the marketing world. Occasionally, a company may want to reformulate an existing product in order to accommodate changing pricing strategies or create a product line extension. In undertaking such an effort, the company might conduct formal consumer tests as a way of ensuring early consumer acceptance while maintaining the product’s established sensory quality. For example, a candy company that makes a popular chocolate bar may need to reformulate its recipe for that bar if the cost of some of its ingredients rises too high for it to be sold at a price that consumers would be willing to pay. In order to avoid raising the price of the chocolate bar, the company might try using more affordable ingredients instead of expensive ones. To ensure that the sensory experience of eating the chocolate bar does not change, the company may conduct tests designed to determine in which ways and to what extent they can alter the recipe of their chocolate bar before consumers notice a difference.

Bibliography

Cherry, Kendra. “Just Noticeable Difference (JND) in Psychology.” Verywell Mind, 26 Dec. 2023, www.verywellmind.com/what-is-the-just-noticeable-difference-2795306. Accessed 18 Jan. 2025.

Cherry, Kendra. “What Is Weber’s Law?” Explore Psychology, 3 Nov. 2023, www.explorepsychology.com/what-is-webers-law. Accessed 18 Jan. 2025.

Dewey, Russ. “Psychophysics.” Psych Web, www.psywww.com/intropsych/ch04-senses/psychophysics.html. Accessed 18 Jan. 2025.

“Just-Noticeable Difference.” Lumen Learning, courses.lumenlearning.com/wm-biology2/chapter/just-noticeable-difference. Accessed 18 Jan. 2025.

Lawson, Euan. "Debrief: Making a Just Noticeable Difference." The British Journal of General Practice, vol. 70, no. 692, 2020, p. 122, doi.org/10.3399/bjgp20X708533. Accessed 18 Jan. 2025.

Milano, Steve. “What Is the Differential Threshold in Marketing?” Chron, smallbusiness.chron.com/differential-threshold-marketing-23983.html. Accessed 18 Jan. 2025.

Morsella, Ezequiel. "Living Below the JND (The 'Just Noticeable Difference'): Psychophysics in Everyday Life." Psychology Today, 21 Apr. 2011, www.psychologytoday.com/us/blog/consciousness-and-the-brain/201104/living-below-the-jnd-the-just-noticeable-difference. Accessed 18 Jan. 2025.

“Psychology 101: Chapter 5.1 Sensation.” AllPsych, allpsych.com/psychology101/sensation. Accessed 18 Jan. 2025.