Racism and social psychology
Racism, as understood within the realm of social psychology, encompasses negative attitudes and behaviors of majority group members towards racial and ethnic minorities. The study of racism integrates aspects of social psychology and sociology, focusing on intergroup relations, cognition, and attitudes. The concept gained prominence in the late 1960s, particularly following the Kerner Commission Report, which highlighted systemic "white racism." Commonly associated with Black-white dynamics in the U.S., racism also extends to relations involving other minority groups, both within and outside the country.
Social psychologists categorize racism into various types, including dominative, aversive, and symbolic racism, reflecting different levels of prejudice and discrimination. Research shows that racial stereotypes are deeply ingrained and can lead to self-fulfilling prophecies, where biased perceptions cause individuals to behave in ways that reinforce those biases. Institutional racism is identified in policies that, while neutral on the surface, result in unequal outcomes for different racial groups. Throughout the years, the study of racism has evolved, revealing that both overt and subtle forms of prejudice persist in society, despite a general decline in the willingness of individuals to openly admit racist views. This complex phenomenon continues to be examined by various disciplines, highlighting the multifaceted nature of racism and its impact on social behavior.
Racism and social psychology
TYPE OF PSYCHOLOGY: Social psychology
Those studying racism examine the phenomenon of negative attitudes and behavior by members of the majority toward those who belong to racial and ethnic minorities. The topic of racism, which straddles the boundaries between social psychology and sociology, is connected with the study of intergroup relations, cognition, and attitudes in general..
Introduction
The social and psychological study of prejudice and discrimination, including prejudice and discrimination against African Americans, has a long history; the term “racism,” however, did not enter the language of social psychology until the publication of the Kerner Commission Report of 1968, which blamed all-pervasive “white racism” for widespread rioting by Black people in American cities. While usually applied to Black-white relations in the United States, the term is also sometimes used with regard to white Americans’ relations with other minority groups, such as Asians or Latinos, or to Black-white relations outside the United States, for example, in Britain, Canada, or South Africa. Most of the studies and research on racism have focused on white racism against Black people in the United States.


Racism is seen by many social psychologists not as mere hatred but as a deep-rooted habit that is hard to change; hence, subvarieties of racism are distinguished. Psychoanalyst Joel Kovel, in his book White Racism: A Psychohistory (1970), distinguishes between dominative racism, the desire to oppress Black people, and aversive racism, the desire to avoid contact with Black individuals. Aversive racism, Samuel L. Gaertner and John Dovidio find, exists among those white people who pride themselves on being unprejudiced. David O. Sears, looking at white peoples’ voting behavior and their political opinions as expressed in survey responses, finds what he calls symbolic racism: a resentment of African Americans for making demands in the political realm that supposedly violate traditional American values. Social psychologist James M. Jones distinguishes three types of racism: individual racism, the prejudice and anti-Black behavior deliberately manifested by individual white people; , the social, economic, and political patterns that impersonally oppress Black persons regardless of the prejudice or lack thereof of individuals; and cultural racism, the tendency of white people to ignore or to denigrate the special characteristics of Black culture.
Where Dovidio and Gaertner find aversive racism, Irwin Katz finds ambivalence. Many white people, he argues, simultaneously see African Americans as disadvantaged (which creates sympathy) and as deviating from mainstream social norms (which creates antipathy). Such ambivalence, Katz contends, leads to exaggeratedly negative reactions to negative behaviors by an African American, but also to exaggeratedly positive reactions to positive behaviors by an African American. He calls this phenomenon ambivalence-induced behavior amplification.
The reasons suggested for individual racism are many. John Dollard, Neal E. Miller, and others, in Frustration and Aggression (1939), see prejudice as the scapegoating of minorities to provide a release for aggression in the face of frustration; in this view, outbursts of bigotry are a natural response to hard economic times. Muzafer and Carolyn Sherif, in Groups in Harmony and Tension (1953) and later works, see prejudice of all sorts as the result of competition between groups. Theodor Adorno and others, in The Authoritarian Personality (1950), view prejudice, whether directed against Black people or against Jews, as reflective of a supposedly fascist type of personality produced by authoritarian child-rearing practices. In Racially Separate or Together? (1971), Thomas F. Pettigrew shows that discriminatory behavior toward Black people, and the verbal expression of prejudices against them, can sometimes flow simply from a white person’s desire to fit in with his or her social group. Finally, both prejudice and discrimination, many psychologists argue, are rooted in those human cognitive processes involved in the formation of .
Racism and Stereotypes
Stereotypes are ideas, often rigidly held, concerning members of a group to which one does not belong. Social psychologists who follow the cognitive approach to the study of racism, such as David L. Hamilton, Walter G. Stephan, and Myron Rothbart, argue that racial stereotyping (the tendency of white people to see Black people in some roles and not in others) arises, like any other kind of stereotyping, from the need of every human being to create some sort of order out of his or her perceptions of the world. Although stereotypes are not entirely impervious to revision or even to shattering in the face of disconfirming instances, information related to a stereotype is more efficiently retained than information unrelated to it. White individuals, it has been found, tend to judge Black people to be more homogeneous than they really are, while being more aware of differences within their own group: This is called the homogeneity hypothesis. White people who are guided by stereotypes may act in such a way as to bring out worse behavior in Black people than would otherwise occur, thus creating a self-fulfilling prophecy.
Why is stereotypical thinking on the part of white people about African Americans so hard to eliminate? The history of race relations in the United States deserves some of the blame. Some mistakes in reasoning common to the tolerant and the intolerant alike—such as the tendency to remember spectacular events and to think of them as occurring more frequently than is really the case (the )—also occur in white peoples’ judgments about members of minority groups. In addition, the social and occupational roles one fills may reinforce stereotypical thinking.
Pettigrew contends that errors—mistakes in explaining the behavior of others—may have an important role to play in reinforcing racial stereotypes. The same behavioral act, Pettigrew argues, is interpreted differently by white people depending on the race of the actor. A positive act by a Black person might be ascribed to situational characteristics (for example, luck, affirmative action programs, or other circumstances beyond one’s control) and thus discounted; a positive act by a white person might be ascribed to personality characteristics. Similarly, a negative act might be ascribed to situational characteristics in the case of a white individual, but to personality characteristics in the case of a Black person. The tendency of white people to view the greater extent of poverty among Black people as solely the result of lack of motivation can be seen as a form of attribution error.
Policy Guides
Institutional racism occurs when policies that are nonracial on their face have differential results for the two races. For example, a stiff educational requirement for a relatively unskilled job may effectively exclude Blacks people, whose educational preparation may be weaker, at least in part because of past racial discrimination. The policy of hiring friends and relatives of existing employees may also exclude Black people, if Black individuals have not historically worked in a particular business. In both cases, the effect is discriminatory even if the intent is not.
Somewhat connected with the concept of institutional racism is Pettigrew’s notion of conformity-induced prejudice and discrimination. A classic example is that of the precivil-rights-era southern United States, where urban restaurant owners, regardless of their personal feelings about Black people, refused them service out of deference to local norms. Another example is the case of the white factory worker who cooperates with Black fellow workers on the job and in union activities but strenuously opposes Black people moving into his neighborhood; norms of tolerance are followed in one context, norms of discrimination in the other.
The concept of symbolic (sometimes called “modern”) racism, a form of covert prejudice said to be characteristic of political conservatives, arose from a series of questions designed to predict whether white Californians would vote against Black political candidates. It has been used to explain opposition to school busing to achieve integration and support for the 1978 California referendum proposition for limiting taxes. John B. McConahay shows that white experimental subjects who score high on the modern racism scale, when faced with hypothetical Black and white job candidates with identical credentials, are more likely than low scorers to give a much poorer rating to the Black candidate’s résumé.
Aversive racism cannot be detected by surveys. Since aversive racists wish to maintain a nonprejudiced , they neither admit to being prejudiced nor discriminate against Black people when social norms clearly forbid it; when the norms are ambiguous, however, they do discriminate. In a New York City experiment, professed liberals and professed conservatives both got telephone calls from individuals identifiable from their speech patterns as either Black or white. At first, the caller said he had the wrong number; if the recipient of the call did not hang up, the caller then asked for help regarding a disabled car. Conservatives were less likely to offer help to the Black person, but liberals were more likely to hang up when they were told by the Black person that a wrong number had been called. In another experiment, white college students proved just as willing to accept help from a Black partner as from a white one when the help was offered. When the subjects had to take the initiative, however, discomfort with the reversal of traditional roles showed up: More asked for help from the white partner than from the Black one.
Both symbolic and aversive, but not dominative, racists manifest ambivalence in their attitudes toward Black people. Katz’s concept of ambivalence-induced behavior amplification has been tested in several experiments. In one experiment, white college student subjects were told to insult two individuals, one Black and one white. After they had done so, they proved, when asked for assistance in a task later on, more willing to help the Black person they had insulted than the white person.
The effect of the availability heuristic in reinforcing stereotypes is seen in the case of a white person who is mugged by a Black criminal. If the victim knows no other Black persons, he or she may well remember this one spectacular incident and forget the many Black people who are law-abiding. The effect of occupational roles in reinforcing stereotypes can be seen in the example of a white police officer who patrols a Black slum neighborhood and jumps to the conclusion that all Black people are criminals.
Experiments on stereotyping indicate that white subjects remember the words or actions of a solo Black person in an otherwise all-white group better than they do the words or actions of one Black individual in a group of several Black people. With a mixed group of speakers, some white and some Black, white experimental subjects proved later to be more likely to confuse the identities of the Black speakers than those of the white speakers, while remembering the race of the former. The self-fulfilling prophecy concept has been tested in experiments with white subjects interviewing supposed job candidates. The white subjects were more ill at ease and inarticulate interviewing a Black candidate than in interviewing a white one; in turn, the Black candidate was more ill at ease than the white one and made more errors.
Since most such experiments use college students as subjects, there is inevitably some doubt about their generalizability to the outside world. Nevertheless, it seems likely that the evidence from social psychology experiments of just how deeply rooted racial bias is among white Americans has played at least some role in leading governments to adopt affirmative action policies to secure fairer treatment of Black people and other minorities in hiring procedures.
History and Developments
Although the study of racism per se began with the racial crisis of the 1960s, the study of prejudice in general goes back much further; as early as the 1920s, Emory Bogardus constructed a social distance scale measuring the degree of intimacy members of different racial and ethnic groups were willing to tolerate with one another. At first, psychologists tended to seek the roots of prejudice in the emotional makeup of the prejudiced individual rather than in the structure of society or in the general patterns of human cognition. For many years, the study of anti-Black prejudice was subsumed under the study of prejudice in general; those biased against Black people were thought to be biased against other groups, such as Jews, as well.
In the years immediately following World War II, American social psychologists were optimistic about the possibilities for reducing or even eliminating racial and ethnic prejudices. Adorno’s The Authoritarian Personality, and The Nature of Prejudice (1954), by Gordon Allport, reflect the climate of opinion of the time. Allport, whose view of prejudice represented a mixture of the psychoanalytic and cognitive approaches, used the term “racism” to signify the doctrines preached by negrophobe political demagogues; he did not see it as a deeply ingrained bad habit pervading the entire society. Pettigrew, who wrote about antiblack prejudice from the late 1950s on, cast doubt on the notion that there was a specific type of personality or pattern of child rearing associated with prejudice. Nevertheless, he long remained in the optimistic tradition, arguing that changing white people’s discriminatory behavior through the enactment of civil rights laws would ultimately change their prejudiced attitudes.
The more frequent use by social psychologists of the term “racism” from the late 1960s onward indicates a growing awareness that bias against Black people, a visible minority, might be harder to uproot than that directed against religious and ethnic minorities. Social psychologists studying racial prejudice shifted their research interest from the open and noisy bigotry most often found among political extremists (for example, the Ku Klux Klan) to the quiet, everyday prejudices of the average apolitical individual. Racial bias against Black people came to be seen as a central, rather than a peripheral, feature of American life.
Responses to surveys taken from the 1940s to the end of the 1970s indicated a steady decline in the percentage of white Americans willing to admit holding racist views. Yet in the 1970s, the sometimes violent white hostility to school busing for integration, and the continuing social and economic gap between Black and white America, gave social psychologists reason to temper their earlier optimism. The contact hypothesis, the notion that contact between different racial groups would reduce prejudice, was subjected to greater skepticism and ever more careful qualification. Janet Schofield, in her field study of a desegregated junior high school, detected a persistence of racial divisions among the pupils; reviewing a number of such studies, Stephan similarly discerned a tendency toward increased interracial tension in schools following desegregation. The pessimism suggested by field studies among younger teenagers was confirmed by experiments conducted in the 1970s and 1980s on college students and adults; such studies demonstrated the existence, even among supposedly nonprejudiced people, of subtle racism and racial stereotyping.
Yet while social psychological experiments contribute to an understanding of the reasons for negative attitudes toward Black people by whites, and for discriminatory behavior toward African Americans even by those white people who believe themselves to be tolerant, they do not by any means provide the complete answer to the riddle of racial prejudice and discrimination. Unlike many other topics in social psychology, racism has also been investigated by journalists, historians, economists, sociologists, political scientists, legal scholars, and even literary critics. The techniques of social psychology—surveys, controlled experiments, and field studies—provide only one window on this phenomenon.
Bibliography
Allport, Gordon W. The Nature of Prejudice. 1954. Reprint. Cambridge: Addison, 1990. Print.
Augoustinos, Martha. “Psychological Perspectives on Racism.” InPsych. Australian Psychological Society, Aug. 2013. Web. 25 June 2014.
Barndt, Joseph. Understanding and Dismantling Racism: The Twenty-First Century Challenge to White America. Minneapolis: Fortress, 2007. Print.
Bell, Derrick. Faces at the Bottom of the Well: The Permanence of Racism. New York: Basic, 1992. Print.
Campbell, Duane. Choosing Democracy: A Practical Guide to Multicultural Education. 4th ed. Boston: Allyn, 2009. Print.
Dovidio, John F., and Samuel L. Gaertner, eds. Prejudice, Discrimination, and Racism. 1986. Rpt. San Diego: Academic, 1992. Print.
Gilroy, Paul. Against Race: Imaging Political Culture beyond the Color Line. Cambridge: Belknap, 2001. Print.
Katz, Irwin. Stigma: A Social Psychological Analysis. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum, 1981. Print.
Katz, Phyllis A., and Dalmas A. Taylor, eds. Eliminating Racism: Profiles in Controversy. New York: Plenum, 1988. Print.
Marsh, Jason, Rodolfo Mendoza-Denton, and Jeremy Adam Smith. Are We Born Racist? New Insights from Neuroscience and Positive Psychology. Boston: Beacon, 2010. Print.
Oshodi, John Egbeazien. History of Psychology in the Black Experience: Perspectives Then and Now—A Psychology in the Perspective of the History of the Africans and People of African Descent. Lanham: UP of America, 2012. Print.
Pettigrew, Thomas F., et al. Prejudice. Cambridge: Belknap, 1982. Print.
Steele, Shelby. The Content of Our Character: A New Vision of Race in America. New York: HarperPerennial, 1998. Print.
Stephan, Walter G., and David Rosenfield. “Racial and Ethnic Stereotypes.” In the Eye of the Beholder: Contemporary Issues in Stereotyping. Ed. Arthur G. Miller. New York: Praeger, 1982. Print.
Sue, Derald Wing, and David Sue. Counseling the Culturally Diverse: Theory and Practice. Hoboken: Wiley, 2013. Print.
Trawalter, Sophie, Gerald D. Higginbotham, and Kyshia Henderson. "Social Psychological Research on Racism and the Importance of Historical Context: Implications for Policy." Current Directions in Psychological Science, vol. 31, no. 6, 2022, DOI: 10.1177/09637214221114092. Accessed 12 Feb. 2023.
Trepagnier, Barbara.Silent Racism: How Well-Meaning White People Perpetuate the Racial Divide.Boulder: Paradigm, 2007. Print.
West, Cornell. Race Matters. 2d ed. Boston: Beacon, 2001. Print.