Social learning according to Albert Bandura

Albert Bandura’s social learning theory, later called social cognitive theory, provides a theoretical framework for understanding and explaining human behavior; the theory embraces an interactional model of causation and accords central roles to cognitive, vicarious, and self-regulatory processes.

TYPE OF PSYCHOLOGY: Personality

Introduction

Social learning theory, later amplified as social cognitive theory by its founder, social psychologist Albert Bandura, provides a unified theoretical framework for analyzing the psychological processes that govern human behavior. Its goal is to explain how behavior develops, how it is maintained, and through what processes it can be modified. It seeks to accomplish this by identifying the determinants of human action and the mechanisms through which they operate.

93872237-60604.jpg

Bandura laid out the conceptual framework of his approach in his book Social Learning Theory (1977). His theory is based on a model of reciprocal determinism. Bandura rejected both the humanist and existentialist position viewing people as free agents and the behaviorist position viewing behavior as controlled by the environment. Rather, external determinants of behavior (such as rewards and punishments) and internal determinants (such as thoughts, expectations, motivation, and beliefs) are considered part of a system of interlocking determinants that influence not only behavior but also the various other parts of the system. In other words, each part of the system—behavior, cognition, and environmental influences—affects each of the other parts. People are neither free agents nor passive reactors to external pressures. Instead, through self-regulatory processes, they have the ability to exercise some measure of control over their own actions. They can affect their behavior by setting goals, arranging environmental inducements, generating cognitive strategies, evaluating goal attainment, and mediating consequences for their actions. Bandura accepted that these self-regulatory functions initially are learned as the result of external rewards and punishments. Their external origin, however, does not invalidate the fact that, once internalized, they in part determine behavior.

Cognitive Mediating Factors

As self-regulation results from symbolic processing of information, Bandura in his theorizing assigned an increasingly prominent role to cognition. This is reflected in his book Social Foundations of Thought and Action: A Social Cognitive Theory (1986), in which he no longer referred to his approach as social learning but as social cognitive theory. People, unlike lower animals, use verbal and nonverbal symbols (language and images) to process information and preserve experiences in the form of cognitive representations. This encoded information serves as a guide for future behavior. Without the ability to use symbols, people would have to solve problems by enacting various alternative solutions until, by trial and error, they learned which ones resulted in rewards or punishments. Through their cognitive abilities, however, people can think through different options, imagine possible outcomes, and guide their behavior by anticipated consequences. Symbolic capabilities provide people with a powerful tool to regulate their own behavior in the absence of external reinforcements and punishments.

According to Bandura, the most central of all mechanisms of self-regulation is self-efficacy, defined as the belief that one has the ability, with one’s actions, to bring about a certain outcome. Self-efficacy beliefs function as determinants of behavior by influencing motivation, thought processes, and emotions in ways that may be self-aiding or self-hindering. Specifically, self-efficacy appraisals determine the goals that people set for themselves, whether they anticipate and visualize scenarios of success or failure, whether they embark on a course of action, how much effort they expend, and how long they persist in the face of obstacles. Self-efficacy expectations are different from outcome expectations. Outcome expectancies are beliefs that a given behavior will result in a certain outcome, while self-efficacy refers to the belief in one’s ability to bring about this outcome. To put it simply, people may believe that something can happen, but whether they embark on a course of action depends on their perceived ability to make it happen.

Relevance to Observation and Modeling

Perhaps the most important contribution of social learning theory to the understanding of human behavior is the concept of vicarious learning, or observational learning, also termed “learning through modeling.” Before the advent of social learning theory, many psychologists assigned a crucial role to the process of reinforcement in learning. They postulated that without performing responses that are followed by reinforcement or punishment, a person cannot learn. In contrast, Bandura asserted that much of social behavior is not learned from the consequences of trial and error but is acquired through symbolic modeling. People watch what other people do and what happens to them as a result of their actions. From such observations, they form ideas of how to perform new behaviors, and later this information guides their actions.

Symbolic modeling is of great significance for human learning because of its enormous efficiency in transmitting information. Whereas trial-and-error learning requires the gradual shaping of the behavior of individuals through repetition and reinforcement, in observational learning, a single model can teach complex behaviors simultaneously to any number of people. According to Bandura, some elaborate and specifically human behavior patterns, such as language, might even be impossible to learn if it were not for symbolic modeling. For example, it seems unlikely that children learn to talk as a result of their parents’ reinforcing each correct utterance they emit. Rather, children probably hear and watch other members of their verbal community talk and then imitate their behavior. In a similar vein, complex behaviors such as driving a car or flying a plane are not acquired by trial and error. Instead, prospective drivers or pilots follow the verbal rules of an instructor until they master the task.

In summary, Bandura’s social learning theory explains human action in terms of the interplay among behavior, cognition, and environmental influences. The theory places particular emphasis on cognitive mediating factors such as self-efficacy beliefs and outcome expectancies. Its greatest contribution to a general theory of human learning has been its emphasis on learning by observation or modeling. Observational learning has achieved the status of a third learning principle, next to classical conditioning and operant conditioning.

Studies of Learning and Performance

From its inception, social learning theory has served as a useful framework for the understanding of both normal and abnormal human behavior. A major contribution that has significant implications for the modification of human behavior is the theory’s distinction between learning and performance. In a now-classic series of experiments, Bandura and his associates teased apart the roles of observation and reinforcement in learning and were able to demonstrate that people learn through mere observation.

In a study on aggression called the Bobo doll experiment, an adult model hit and kicked a life-size inflated clown doll (a “Bobo” doll), with children watching the attack in person or on a television screen. Other children watched the model perform some innocuous behavior. Later, the children were allowed to play in the room with the Bobo doll. All children who had witnessed the aggression, either in person or on television, attacked the doll, while those who had observed the model’s innocuous behavior did not display aggression toward the doll. Moreover, it was clearly shown that the children modeled their aggressive behaviors after the adult. Those who had observed the adult sit on the doll and hit its face, or kick the doll, or use a hammer to pound it imitated exactly these behaviors. Thus, the study accomplished its purpose by demonstrating that observational learning occurs in the absence of direct reinforcement.

In a related experiment, Bandura showed that expected consequences, while not relevant for learning, play a role in performance. A group of children watched a film of an adult model behaving aggressively toward a Bobo doll and being punished, while another group observed the same behavior with the person being rewarded. When the children subsequently were allowed to play with the Bobo doll, those who had watched the model being punished displayed fewer aggressive behaviors toward the doll than those who had seen the model being rewarded. When the experimenter then offered a reward to the children for imitating the model, however, all children, regardless of the consequences they had observed, attacked the Bobo doll. This showed that all children had learned the aggressive behavior from the model but that observing the model being punished served as an inhibiting factor until it was removed by the promise of a reward. Again, this study showed that children learn without reinforcement, simply by observing how others behave. Whether they then engage in the behavior, however, depends on the consequences that they expect will result from their actions.

Disinhibitory Effects

Models not only teach people novel ways of thinking and behaving but also can strengthen or weaken inhibitions. Seeing models punished may inhibit similar behavior in observers, while seeing models carry out feared or forbidden actions without negative consequences may reduce their inhibitions.

The most striking demonstrations of the disinhibitory effects of observational learning come from therapeutic interventions based on modeling principles. Bandura, in his book Principles of Behavior Modification (1969), showed how social learning theory can provide a conceptual framework for the modification of a wide range of maladaptive behaviors. For example, a large number of laboratory studies of subjects with a severe phobia of snakes showed that phobic individuals can overcome their fear of reptiles when fearless adult models demonstrate how to handle a snake and directly assist subjects in coping successfully with whatever they dread.

Self-Efficacy Mechanism

In later elaborations, the scope of social learning theory was amplified to include self-efficacy theory. Self-efficacy is now considered the principal mechanism of behavior change, in that all successful interventions are assumed to operate by strengthening a person’s self-perceived efficacy to cope with difficulties.

How can self-efficacy be strengthened? Research indicates that it is influenced by four sources of information. The most important influence comes from performance attainments, with successes heightening and failures lowering perceived self-efficacy. Thus, having people enact and master a difficult task most powerfully increases their efficacy percepts. A second influence comes from vicarious experiences. Exposing people to models works because seeing people similar to oneself successfully perform a difficult task raises one’s own efficacy expectations. Verbal persuasion is a third way of influencing self-efficacy. Convincing people that they have the ability to perform a task can encourage them to try harder, which indeed may lead to successful performance. Finally, teaching people coping strategies to lower emotional arousal can also increase self-efficacy. If subsequently they approach a task more calmly, then their likelihood of succeeding at it may increase.

Bandura and his associates conducted a series of studies to test the idea that vastly different modes of influence all improve coping behavior by strengthening self-perceived efficacy. Individuals with severe snake phobias received interventions based on enactive, vicarious, cognitive, or emotive treatment (a method of personality change that incorporates cognitive, emotional, and behavioral strategies, designed to help resist tendencies to be irrational, suggestible, and conforming) modalities. The results confirmed that the degree to which people changed their behavior toward the reptiles was closely associated with increases in self-judged efficacy, regardless of the method of intervention. It is now widely accepted among social learning theorists that all effective therapies ultimately work by strengthening people’s self-perceptions of efficacy.

Theoretical Influences

Social learning theory was born into a climate in which two competing and diametrically opposed schools of thought dominated psychology. On one hand, psychologists who advocated psychodynamic theories postulated that human behavior is governed by motivational forces operating in the form of largely unconscious needs, drives, and impulses. These impulse theories tended to give circular explanations, attributing behavior to inner causes that were inferred from the very behavior they were supposed to cause. They also tended to provide explanations after the fact, rather than predicting events, and had very limited empirical support.

On the other hand, there were various types of behavior theory that shifted the focus of the causal analysis from hypothetical internal determinants of behavior to external, publicly observable causes. Behaviorists were able to show that actions commonly attributed to inner causes could be produced, eliminated, and reinstated by manipulating the antecedent (stimulus) and consequent (reinforcing) conditions of the person’s external environment. This led to the proposition that people’s behavior is caused by factors residing in the environment.

Social learning theory presents a theory of human behavior that to some extent incorporates both viewpoints. According to Bandura, people are neither driven by inner forces nor buffeted by environmental stimuli; instead, psychological functioning is best explained in terms of a continuous reciprocal interaction of internal and external causes. This assumption, termed reciprocal determinism, became one of the dominant viewpoints in psychology.

An initial exposition of social learning theory is presented in Bandura and Richard H. Walters’s text Social Learning and Personality Development (1963). This formulation drew heavily on the procedures and principles of operant and classical conditioning. In his later book, Principles of Behavior Modification, Bandura placed much greater emphasis on symbolic events and self-regulatory processes. He argued that complex human behavior cannot be satisfactorily explained by the narrow set of learning principles that behaviorists had derived from animal studies. He incorporated principles derived from developmental, social, and cognitive psychology into social learning theory.

Evolution of Theoretical Development

During the 1970s, psychology had grown increasingly cognitive. This development is reflected in Bandura’s 1977 book Social Learning Theory, which presents self-efficacy theory as the central mechanism through which people control their own behavior. Over the following decade, the influence of cognitive psychology on Bandura’s work grew stronger. In his 1986 book Social Foundations of Thought and Action, he finally disavowed his roots in learning theory and renamed his approach “social cognitive theory.” This theory accords central roles to cognitive, vicarious, self-reflective, and self-regulatory processes.

Social learning/social cognitive theory became the dominant conceptual approach within the field of behavior therapy. It has provided the conceptual framework for numerous interventions for a wide variety of psychological disorders, as well as for examining things such as child development, business management, education, and aggressive behavior and criminality.

In the twenty-first century, Bandura related social cognitive theory to mass communication, explaining how behavior can disseminate through society through psychological factors, and applied it to health promotion and global issues in the chapter "The Social and Policy Impact of Social Cognitive Theory," from the collaborative book Social Psychology and Evaluation (2011). In 2016, Bandura published Moral Disengagement: How People Do Harm and Live with Themselves. In the book, he used social cognitive theory to explore how people justify harmful actions, explaining how people can disengage from the harm they do if the action is done for what they perceive is a worthy cause. His 2023 book, Social Cognitive Theory: An Agentic Perspective on Human Nature, looked even further into Bandura's years of research and explored social cognitive theory in relation to human nature and human agency.

In recognition of his work, Bandura was honored with the Award for Distinguished Scientific Contributions to Psychology from the American Psychological Foundation in 1980.

Bibliography

Akers, Ronald. A General Theory of Crime and Deviance. Transaction, 2009.

Bandura, Albert. Principles of Behavior Modification. Holt, 1969.

Bandura, Albert. A Social Cognitive Theory. Prentice, 1986.

Bandura, Albert. Social Learning Theory. Prentice, 1977.

Bandura, Albert. Moral Disengagement: How People Do Harm and Live with Themselves. Worth, 2016.

Bandura, Albert. Social Cognitive Theory: An Agentic Perspective on Human Nature. Wiley & Sons, 2023.

Bingham, Tony, and Marcia Conner. A Guide to Transforming Organization through Social Media. Berett, 2010.

Cherry, Kendra. "How Social Learning Theory Works." VeryWellMind, 15 July 2024, www.verywellmind.com/social-learning-theory-2795074. Accessed 17 July 2024.

Evans, Richard I. Albert Bandura, the Man and His Ideas—A Dialogue. Praeger, 1989.

Feist, Jess, and Gregory J. Feist. Theories of Personality. 8th ed. McGraw, 2012.

Gawronski, Bertram, and Galen V. Bodenhausen, eds. Theory and Explanation in Social Psychology. Guilford, 2014.

Jaishankar, K. International Perspectives on Crime and Justice. Cambridge Scholars, 2009.

Monks, Claire P., and Iain Coyne. Bullying in Different Contexts. Cambridge UP, 2011.

Nickerson, Charlotte. "Albert Bandura's Social Cognitive Theory." Simply Psychology, 2 Feb. 2024, www.simplypsychology.org/social-cognitive-theory.html. Accessed 17 July 2024.

Zimmerman, Barry J., and Dale H. Schunk, eds. A Century of Contributions. Erlbaum, 2003.