Hung juries

SIGNIFICANCE: The controversy over the phenomenon of hung juries has been the catalyst for empirical research and jury reform.

The occurrence of hung juries within the criminal justice system has long been a controversial and intriguing phenomenon. Hung juries have remained an issue of concern over time because of their potential negative consequences, which include the emotional impact on the victims and families and the costs of retrying cases.

There are many explanations as to why jury members fail to convict or acquit a defendant. A hung jury may be the result of individual, group, or evidentiary factors. Many critics of the jury system propose that hung juries result from failures within the group interaction during deliberation. Hung juries have been attributed to jurors’ poor intelligence, personality eccentricities, corruption, difficulty with evidence comprehension, and outright refusal to follow the law (such as jury nullification ). In addition, ambiguous or inadequate evidence presentation by the attorneys may also result in a jury deadlocking.

Understanding the actual factors contributing to hung juries is challenging. For example, many jurisdictions do not maintain systematic records of jury trials that hang because they occur so infrequently. More specifically, jury deliberations have long been viewed as a “black box” demanding absolute confidentiality. As a result, early research on juries, and more specifically on hung juries, was scarce before the mid-1970s. Groundbreaking and early empirical research was conducted on the American jury by Harry Kalven, Jr. and Hans Zeisel. Their study briefly addressed the issue of hung juries and found that hung juries occurred at a rate of 5.5 percent, or roughly one in twenty.

The catalyst for empirical research on the jury system and hung juries was the U.S. Supreme Court decision in Apodaca v. Oregon (1972). In Apodaca, the Supreme Court decided unanimous verdicts were not constitutionally mandated in noncapital cases and that a majority vote by jurors could convict or acquit a defendant. The Court’s reasoning was that allowing majority-rule verdicts would increase the efficiency of the court process and decrease the cost of a jury trial, with little or no negative consequences on the quality of jury deliberations.

Subsequent research confirmed that the rate of hung juries and the length of jury deliberations decreased when majority-rule verdicts were used. However, empirical research also revealed that nonunanimous verdicts decreased the quality of deliberation and decreased juror satisfaction with the deliberation process. For example, once a majority verdict was met, the minority or dissenting opinions no longer needed to be considered.

Bibliography

Abramson, Jeffery. We, the Jury: The Jury System and the Ideal of Democracy. Cambridge: Harvard UP, 1994. Print.

Gelman, Elijah N. "Hung Out to Try: A Rule 29 Revision to Stop Hung Jury Retrials." Northwestern University Law Review, vol. 118, no. 4, 24 Jan. 2024, scholarlycommons.law.northwestern.edu/nulr/vol118/iss4/5/. Accessed 5 July 2024.

Hannaford-Agor, Paula L., Valerie P. Hans, Nicole L. Mott, and G. Thomas Munsterman. Are Hung Juries a Problem? Williamsburg: National Center for State Courts, 2002. Print.

Kalven, Harry, and Hans Zeisel. The American Jury. Boston: Little, Brown, 1966. Print.

Roth, Tanya. "What Happens When a Jury Is Deadlocked?" FindLaw, 28 May 2024, www.findlaw.com/legalblogs/criminal-defense/what-happens-when-a-jury-is-deadlocked/. Accessed 5 July 2024.